Etikk i praksis. Nordic Journal of Applied Ethics (2024), 18(2), 7-10 |
http://dx.doi.org/10.5324/eip.v18i2.6279 |
First publication date: 27 December
2024 |
Introduction Constructive
inquiry amidst fear and polarization
As
some
issues divide opinion and create
controversy, reasoned and critical inquiry
could help develop empathetic understanding
among people who need to work together.
Whether climate change, public health, or
socio-economic disparities, many debates
polarize due to wide-ranging fear and
uncertainty about the future. Above all,
these feelings, especially the sentiment of
fear, create deeply held impressions on the
public view and political behavior.
Widespread misinformation acts both as fuel
and symptom of fear in the face of an
uncertain future. We need to address this by
promoting informed and rational civic
conversation. Understanding
Fear and Its Impact on Political
Behavior
Fear
as
an emotion is uniquely capable of influencing
personal and collective decision-making.
According to research (McGrew et al., 2018;
Nabi et al., 2018), fear increases partisan
polarization, reinforcing confirmation biases
and deepening ideological cleavages.
Individuals retire to their "echo chambers,"
looking for information that assures them in
their belief systems and avoid others'
messages. In this process, misinformation gets
spread, and critical inquiry—real critical
inquiry to solve problems with depth and
nuance—is hindered. Studies (Hibbing et al.,
2014) have found that the group of individuals
exposed to a fear-inducing stimulus are more
likely afterward to support authoritarian
policies or leaders promising stability and
protection. This may give a feeling of
temporary security but most often sacrifices a
much larger reward because of a long-term
viable solution. Policy goals, for instance,
set by the fear of economic insecurity and/or
immigration can provide the breeding ground
for unsustainable social cleavages and impede
progress toward problem-solving in cooperation
on a global scale.
Reason
and
critical inquiry could work against the
effects of fearful, reactive
decision-making, at least from a normative
perspective. We need to empirically test the
effectiveness of this prescription though.
If effective, reason and critical inquiry
could open up possibilities to challenge
assumptions, weigh available evidence, and
consider other viewpoints-important
precursors to well-considered, adaptive
decisions. Similar to media literacy
programs and even critical thinking courses
in general, training in critical inquiry
could enhance capacities to analyze credible
information against misinformation. Such
capabilities might reduce susceptibility to
the fear mongering narrative (McGrew et al.,
2018).
To
this
end, it is important to understand the
psychological mechanisms through which
emotions like fear influence political
behavior. Drawing together insights from
psychology, sociology, and political science
will help us devise strategies that channel
emotions into constructive action rather
than divisive rhetoric. For example,
reframing socio-economic security concerns
in terms of opportunities for collective
growth, rather than as threats, can motivate
more cooperative and forward-thinking
solutions (Nabi et al., 2018).
Probably
one
of the most pervasive sources of fear in
modern society is uncertainty about
socio-economic security. The rising gap in
income, job replacement by automation, and
risks linked to climate change further raise
the uncertainty, making those who are
already vulnerable to socio-economic risks
more vulnerable to misinformation and
scapegoating. It is not just reassurance on
the surface that will handle such fears but
a deep transformation for equity,
resilience, and transparency.
Equally
important
is countering misinformation. This is a
process of not only debunking with facts,
but also building an environment of
information that prioritizes accuracy,
context, and access to information.
Platforms and policymaking will need to lead
in ways that ensure public dialogue is
evidence-based and not built on
sensationalism.
Navigating
an increasingly uncertain future requires
instilling a culture of reasoned inquiry and
critical thinking. It will be by addressing
the psychological and socioeconomic roots of
fear that we equip people with the ability to
enter informed argument and constructive
debate. We thereby prepare the ground upon
which sustainable solutions that bridge
ideological divides to create a future of
mutual understanding, collaboration, and
resilience may be achieved.
In the article “What’s
the
beef with cultivated meat?”, Henrik
Andersson and Andrés G. Garcia question
whether cultivated meat is truly an ethical
solution to traditional meat production.
While supporters argue that it could
significantly reduce environmental harm and
lessen the suffering of animals (Heidemann
et al., 2020), the authors maintain that
ethical concerns remain unresolved. Critics
from a consequentialist viewpoint suggest
that resources devoted to cultivated meat
might be better used to tackle pressing
global issues like hunger or climate change
(Specht, 2020). From a deontological
perspective, the use of animal-derived
components, such as fetal bovine serum,
raises ethical concerns about violating
animals’ rights, particularly their bodily
integrity (Van der Valk et al., 2018).
Virtue ethics adds another layer,
questioning whether consuming cultivated
meat tacitly endorses exploitative practices
and stifles moral progress (Alvaro, 2019).
Symbolic concerns, such as parallels to
eating human flesh replicas (Fischer &
Ozturk, 2017), further suggest that
cultivated meat might reinforce associations
between animals and consumption. Despite
these critiques, one could ask the question
whether cultivated meat might serve as a
transitional option for those hesitant to
fully embrace veganism. The authors argue
that efforts could be better directed toward
promoting vegan diets and removing barriers
to their widespread adoption. Before we can
do this, we need to critically address
unresolved issues related to how cultivated
meat fits within diverse cultural contexts,
strategies for phasing out factory farming,
and its implications for global food
justice. There is a need to continue
discussing these ethical dilemmas alongside
practical considerations, exploring whether
cultivated meat could support a larger shift
toward sustainable and equitable food
systems (Rosenfeld & Tomiyama, 2022). As
the article challenges the idea that
cultivated meat is a definitive ethical
solution, we should critically examine other
unresolved issues.
Sean
Clancy's
paper, "Jet
Travel
and Desert", gives a rather
contrarian look at climate ethics and, more
precisely, the moral plausibility of
lifelong travel pledges to reduce greenhouse
gas emissions. Indeed, according to Clancy,
such pledges are usually morally
impermissible insofar as they would deny
individuals their due deserts. This
perspective opens up a new tension between
individual virtue and moral desert in
applied ethics, adding significantly to the
current debate on personal responsibility in
climate change mitigation.
Clancy
argues
against the belief that personal emissions
reductions, such as giving up air travel, are
an effective way of combating climate change.
Using the work of Sinnott-Armstrong and
Kingston, he argues that if an action does not
make a difference in the world, it cannot be
morally justified. He further says that travel
pledges require the personal costs of reduced
well-being to be virtuous, yet this is not
rewarded. He concludes that this
inconsistency—the fact that individuals act
selflessly without receiving what is owed to
them—makes such pledges morally questionable. Indeed, further research is needed. We need to find out how we could reconcile the individual virtues in a manner to bring on system-level changes. We need to consider alternative understandings of our individual and collective roles to help in striking a balance between responsibilities for climate ethics. Works of this sort from Clancy further galvanize discussion on all the nuances accompanying personal sacrifices towards global sustainability. Acknowledgments We would like to thank the contributors as well as the anonymous reviewers for this Special Issue. Their meticulous reading and critical comments helped make the submitted papers more accessible to a wider audience. We are grateful for the guest editors for putting together this Special Issue of the Nordic Journal of Applied Ethics. Note The editors of this journal would like to note that, for this Special Issue honoring the contributions of Icelandic philosopher Vilhjálmur Árnason to applied ethics, we have adopted the Icelandic citation custom of using the first names, rather than their last names, which is the usual practice in academic publications outside of Iceland. As such, we will refer to Icelandic authors by their first names both in in-text citations and in the reference list. In Icelandic naming conventions, last names are patronymic or matronymic and are not used in the same way as surnames in other cultures. Therefore, we believe it is more appropriate to follow the Icelandic custom and use first names for citation purposes. We also see this as an opportunity to acknowledge and celebrate the diverse practices our authors bring to the Nordic Journal of Applied Ethics. References Banks,
A. J., & Valentino, N. A. (2012).
Emotional substrates of ideological
polarization: Fear, anger, and anxiety in
political decision making. American
Journal of Political Science, 56(2),
267–283. CrossRef
Hetherington,
M., & Suhay, E. (2011). Authoritarianism,
threat, and Americans’ support for the war on
terror. American Journal of Political
Science, 55(3), 546–560. CrossRef
McGrew,
S., Ortega, T., Breakstone, J., &
Wineburg, S. (2018). The challenge that's
bigger than fake news: Teaching critical
thinking about digital content. American
Educator, 41(3), 4–9. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1156387.pdf
Nabi,
R. L., Gustafson, A., & Jensen, R. (2018).
Framing climate change: Exploring the role of
emotion in generating advocacy behavior. Science
Communication, 40(4), 442–468. CrossRef
Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B.,
& Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in
negativity bias underlie variations in
political ideology. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 37(3),
297–350. CrossRef
Alvaro, C. (2019). Lab-grown meat
and veganism: A virtue-oriented perspective. Journal of
Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 32(2),
127–141. CrossRef
Fischer,
B., & Ozturk, B. (2017). Facsimiles of
flesh. Journal
of Applied Philosophy, 34(4),
489–497. CrossRef
Heidemann,
M. S., Molento, C. F. M., Reis, G. G., &
Phillips, C. J. C. (2020). Uncoupling meat
from animal slaughter and its impacts on
human-animal relationships. Frontiers
in Psychology, 11, 1824. CrossRef
Rosenfeld,
D. L., & Tomiyama, A. J. (2022). Why
people feel disgusted by cultured meat. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 80,
101758. CrossRef
Specht,
L. (2020). Analysis of culture medium costs
for cultivated meat. The Good Food Institute.
https://gfi.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/07/Medium-Analysis-White-Paper.pdf Van der Valk, J., et al. (2018). Fetal bovine serum (FBS): past–present–future. ALTEX, 35(1), 1–20. CrossRef
|